INFLUENCES ON STUDENT LEARNING
John Hattie
Inaugural Lecture: Professor of Education
University of Auckland
August 2, 1999
I welcome the opportunity to speak to my colleagues
and friends on the research I wish to undertake during my next years here at
the University of Auckland. This is my third Inaugural lecture since I became a
Professor 15 years ago, hence I have learnt to take this opportunity seriously
and cast a vision, as there are so few opportunities to talk to colleagues
one’s own esoterica and passions. Maybe it is that I just need to do the same
thing three times to at least say something, and I know my family has therefore
renamed these my ‘Ignorable Lectures’. I do know that the fact most remembered
from my first was that it was the first time that the chair Michael Scriven and
the presenter presented an Inaugural and both did not wear ties. The fact from
the second in North Carolina was that it was scheduled at the same time as the
Simpson jury read its verdict.
We know that students in lectures learn most in the
first 8 minutes, only recall three things at most after one hour, and that if
the content does not shake their prior beliefs they file away the fascinating
facts in the deepest recesses of their brain, if at all. Hence, my aim tonight
is to wax lyrical about my research projects, to build a theme around my
measurement issues that have impact on students, to explain that about which I
have much passion, and to give you three fascinating facts.
Too much is known
There is so much known about what makes a
difference in the classroom. A glance at the journals on the shelves of most
libraries, my colleagues’ shelves, and on web pages would indicate that the
state of knowledge in the discipline of education is healthy. The worldwide
picture certainly is one of plenty. We now can have a library solely consisting
of Handbooks, most of which can not be held in the hand. Given this bounty, it
would seem remarkable that there is much left to say. That never stopped an
academic.
There are at least three things to say:
• We constantly bemoan the lack of integrative
reviews and typically
blame methodology;
• The methodological war hinders us seeing the
stories in each others
work;
• We have few integrative, bold and verifiable
models of student learning, preferring to study one variable at a time,
occasionally investigating interactive effects, too often look at classrooms
through unfocused eyes, and rarely combine the texture of the learning
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 1experience with the rigour of asking whether
there is an impact on student learning. This lack has not stopped us studying
our pet beliefs. I must note that this love of pet belief is matched in
classrooms, and herein are the three truths of research in classrooms:
1. Teachers/Researchers have models of learning
that are rarely externally elaborated or asked for
2. Teachers/Researchers seek evidence to buttress
their models of learning and thus rarely seek to refute them or introduce major
changes
3. There are no bad Teachers/Researchers.
We all seek positive evidence in that which we
love. Teachers/Researchers, like lovers, are often blind.
Hence, we have a school community peopled with
teachers with self-fulfilling prophecies, all believing they are doing a good
job, and with models of learning rarely based on any other evidence than
"it works for me". As well we have an educational research community
peopled with academics chasing their pet theory, promoting their own
methodology while passing each other in corridors, rarely asking for negative
evidence, and pushing with passion that "if only the teachers would do
this, or know that". Both educational communities work behind closed
doors, coming out to discuss kids, curricula, accountability, and each other,
but rarely discussing the fundamental tenets about their teaching that leads to
positive impacts on student learning.
I believe the first step to resolve this dilemma is
basic, simple, and powerful, and has taken me 10 years to build, and am still
going. My three principles are:
• We need to make relative statements about what
impacts on student work
• We need estimates of magnitude as well as
statistical significance – it is not good enough to say that this works because
lots of people use it etc., but that this works because of the magnitude of
impact
• We need to be building a model based on these
relative magnitudes of effects.
A model of effects on school and teaching A model
of teaching and learning is proposed that is based on three postulates. In this
session I wish to outline the model and the kinds of data that are being used
to support it.
• That achievement is enhanced to the degree that
students and teachers set and communicate appropriate, specific and challenging
goals
• That achievement is enhanced as a function of
feedback.
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 2• That increases in
student learning involves more than surface and
deep learning but also follows a
reconceptualisation of information. A model of effects on school and teaching
• Achievement is enhanced to the degree that
students and teachers set and communicate appropriate, specific and challenging
goals
• Achievement is enhanced as a function of
feedback
• Increases in student learning involves not only
surface and deep learning but also a reconceptualisation of information. Let me
defend these simple propositions.
MEASURING THE EFFECTS OF SCHOOLING
A major purpose of this Inaugural is to show how
the data from the past 30 years of educational research can be used to assess
the effects of innovations and schooling, to provide insights for future
innovation, and provide reasons why different advocates make apparently
contrary claims about the effectiveness of schooling (even using the same data)
by demonstrating how different points of comparison are used by each group. The
beginning of the answer to the question as to the effects of schooling is to
ask, "What are the 'typical' effects of schooling?" and then to use
this typical effect as a benchmark. The problem is how to ascertain "typical
effects" given the myriad of effects on schools, different teachers,
subjects, school administration systems, ages of students, and other moderators
such as gender, prior ability, quality of instruction and teaching styles. The first requirement is a continuum on which
the effects of schooling, including the typical effect, can be summarized where
0 means that there is no effect from introducing some teaching package,
innovation, or effect on schooling. A negative effect indicates that the
innovation has a decreased effect on achievement, and a positive effect
indicates that the innovation has as increased effect on achievement. For the
present, the model is constrained to achievement outcomes, but the continuum
can be generalized to other outcomes of schooling, and I have also undertaken a
similar continuum for special education students.
The next requirement is to formulate an appropriate
scale and it is recommended that the scale is expressed in effect-sizes. An
effect-size provides a common expression of the magnitude of study outcomes for
all types of outcome variables, such as school achievement. An effect-size of
1.0 indicates an increase of one standard deviation, typically associated with
advancing children's achievement by one year, improving the rate of learning by
50%, or a correlation between some variable (e.g., amount of homework) and
achievement of approximately .50. When implementing a new program, an
effect-size of 1.0 would mean that approximately 95% of outcomes positively
enhance achievement, or average students receiving that treatment would exceed
84% of students not receiving that treatment.
Cohen (1977) argued that an effect-size of 1.0
would be regarded as large, blatantly Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 3obvious,
grossly perceptible, and he provided examples such as the difference between
mean IQ of PhD graduates and high school students (we hope). The use of
effect-sizes highlights the importance of the magnitude of differences, which
is contrary to the usual emphasis on statistical significance.
Cohen (1990) has commented that "under the
sway of the Fisherian scheme (or dependence on statistical significance), there
has been little consciousness of how big things are. ... science is inevitably
about magnitudes" and the use of effect-sizes makes a welcome force
towards the accumulation of knowledge" (p. 1310).
For example, it was possible to locate 31
meta-analyses, 17952 studies, and 352 effect-sizes studies that investigated
the effects of introducing computers on students' achievement (see Hattie,
1986). Using meta-analysis, these effects can be statistically synthesized to
ascertain an overall effect as well as assessing the influence of differing
groups of students (e.g., males versus females), different uses of computers,
subject areas, and so on. The average effect-size across these 557 studies was
.31.
Thus, compared to classes without computers, the
use of computers was associated with advancing children's achievement by
approximately three months, improving the rate of learning by 15%, about 65% of
the effects were positive (that is, improved achievement), thus 35% of the
effects were zero or negative, and the average student achievement level after
using computers exceeded 62% of the achievement levels of the students not
using computers. An effect-size of .31 would not, according to Cohen (1977), be
perceptible to the naked observational eye, and would be approximately
equivalent to the difference between the height of a 5'11" and a 6'0"
person. Of course, this is only an overall effect-size from introducing
computers, although contrary to many beliefs the variability around these
effects is quite small. There are many important moderators. For example, the
effects decrease with age: primary students gain most (effect-size = .48), secondary
students have medium gains (effect-size = .32), and college and university
students gain least (effect-size = .25); there are differences in effect-sizes
on
achievement between males and females in secondary
but not elementary classes (see Fitzgerald, Hattie, & Hughes, 1985; Hattie
& Fitzgerald, 1987).
Compared to not having computers in schools
(effect=0) computing can help. Herein lie three major themes:
• we can synthesis seemingly disparate studies to
make an overall conclusion;
• diverse studies can be systematically integrated
to address the question of the magnitude of effect;
• we can then determine those variables that may
moderate this overall effect.
There is a critical step missing here to build a
defensible model of student learning. It will turn out that there are very few
influences in education that are overall negative; hence it seems there is
evidence for every teachers’ pet theme – and this is too close to reality. So
to the missing step. Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 4The missing step is that
the comparison should NOT be to zero point; we must not compare having
computers to not having computers, we must not compare ourselves as teachers to
not having us, but we must compare innovations to other innovations. I suggest
to you that the synthesis of many studies can provide the normative effect to
which other can be compared. Over the past 10 years I have been accumulating
studies, and now have 337 meta-analyses, 200,000 effect-sizes from 180,000
studies, representing approximately 50+ million students, and covering almost
all methods of innovation.
The key question is "What is the typical
effect of schooling"? The answer is derived from averaging the effects
across the 357 meta-analyses and is .40 (with a se= .05). Most innovations that
are introduced in schools improve achievement by about .4 of a standard
deviation. This is the benchmark figure and provides a "standard"
from which to judge effects. A comparison based on typical, real-world effects
rather than based on the strongest cause possible, or with the weakest cause
imaginable. At minimum, this continuum provides a method for measuring the
effects of schooling.
The typical effect does not mean that merely
placing a teacher in front of a class would lead to an improvement of .4
standard deviations. Some deliberate attempt to change, improve, plan, modify,
or innovate is involved. The best available estimate as to the effects of
schooling not based on innovations is from the National Assessment of
Educational Progress (NAEP) data bank (Johnson & Zwick, 1990). NAEP
surveyed what students in American schools knew and could do in the subject
areas of reading, writing, civics, US history, mathematics and science. The
students were sampled at ages 9, 13 and 17, and the testing has been repeated
every two years. The average effect-size across the six subject areas was .24
per year, which Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 5indicates that the effects of
innovations is (.40-.24 =) .16 standard deviations above and beyond the teacher
effects. A further contention of many researchers is that maturation alone can
account for much of the enhancement of learning. The effect of maturation is
probably about one-third of the achievement effect (.10; see Cahen & Davis,
1987). Schooling does enhance learning above the influences of maturation.
Hence my three normative comparison points:
Normative comparison points
• student maturation .10
• a teacher in front of a classroom .24
• innovations in schooling .40
The obvious – schooling makes a difference –
contrary to the Coleman study, and possible contrary to the Harris argument
about the power of peers over parents and teachers – although I keenly await
the results of my colleagues Ian W, Mike T, Judy P, Charlotte and Irene and
others who are working on the peer influences literature review as part of a
grant, at present.
COMPUTERS.
Let me return to computers and now
compare their effects with other influences — then they are just in there. So,
out with the computer salespeople who push computers as the magic answer — yes,
compared to not having them they have an influence, but compared to other
influences a different conclusion can be made — there are not so influential.
Let me show you an example of the innovations that are below the typical
effect.
RETENTION
The effect is among the very lowest of many
possible innovations and it can be vividly noted that retention is
overwhelmingly disastrous across many educational interventions at enhancing
academic achievement. This topic is fascinating to me. Last year I was an
expert witness for the NAACP in a Federal Court Case arguing that retention had
a particularly invidious impact on African American children, I have been
awe-stuck at the number of teachers who fundamentally believe retention is
justified ("if you could only give me more time"), and reiterate to
you that these defenders compare the potential impact of holding the student
back a year because of a belief that the difference between the start of the
grade and end of the grade could increase not appreciating that the comparison
is the start of the grade with
the performance of similar students who are now
tested at a higher grade. The groups of non-promoted/retained students scored
.15 to 26 standard deviation units lower than the promoted comparison groups
on the various outcome measures:
• After one year the retained groups were scoring
. 45 standard deviation unit lower than the comparison groups who had gone on to
the next grade and in many cases were being tested on more advanced material. Each
subsequent year this difference became larger with the difference reaching .83
standard deviation unit for measures taken four or more years after the time of
retention.
• Being retained one year almost doubled a
student’s likelihood of dropping out, while failing twice almost guaranteed it.
• The negative effects are pervasive over all
academic and personal educational outcomes, and at all ages (including
kindergarten)
• There is a consistently negative picture of the
association between retention and race, gender, SES, and school outcomes.
Those who continue to retain pupils at grade level
do so despite cumulative research evidence showing that the potential for
negative effects consistently outweighs positive outcomes.
It
would be difficult to find another educational practice on which the evidence
is so unequivocally negative (House, 1989).
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 7 CLASS SIZE The
research on the effects of class size has been among the more voluminous in
educational research with very systematic findings:
• Achievement, attitude, teacher morale, student
satisfaction gains are appreciable in smaller classes, so long as we recognise
that small classes mean 10-15, as there are negligible gains between 40 to 20 students
per class.
• This effect was the same for primary and
secondary schools, across all subjects, and across various ability levels.
• There is little evidence that instruction methods
change when class size is reduced, although a large part of improvement can be
explained by improvements in student task engagement.
Reducing class sizes from the 30’s to the 20’s is
in the right direction, but there is little support for the claim that there
are increases in student achievement or satisfaction, or teacher attitude or
morale. Only when the class size reduces to 15 or below are there appreciable
positive benefits.
TELEVISION
This is one of the few non-linear effects. Less
than 10 hours per week is associated with increases in achievement, more than
10 hours with decreases – but show me a New Zealand child who watches less than
10 hours per week. And this table provides examples of influences above the
average.
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 8Five overall
findings from these positive effects:
• critical innovations
• feedback
• setting of appropriate, specific and challenging
goals
• It is what some teachers do that makes the
difference; and
• The introduction of most teaching and school
influences merely impacts on the probability of the presence of feedback and
challenging goals.
First, innovation is the theme underlying most of
these effects. That is, a constant and deliberate attempt to improve the
quality of learning on behalf of the system, principal and teacher typically
relates to improved achievement.
The implementation of innovations probably captures
the enthusiasm of the teacher implementing the innovation and the excitement of
the students attempting something innovative. Often this has been explained as
an experimental artifact in terms of a Hawthorne effect. No matter the reason,
it appears that innovation per se can have positive effects on students’ achievement.
Teachers who constantly question "How are I going", who wish to
verify that their methods are having impacts on student learning are the prerequisites
for excellence.
I highlight this innovation and willingness to try
new methods particularly as too many classes involve listening, mimicking,
copying, and learning how to survive the information overload. Second, the most
powerful single moderator that enhances achievement is feedback. The simplest
prescription for improving education must be "dollops of feedback" --
providing information how and why the child understands and misunderstands, and
what directions the student must take to improve.
The most fundamental component of teaching is
imparting information to students, assessing and evaluating the students
understanding of this information, and then matching the next teaching act to
the present understandings of the student.
This is not feedback in the behavioural sense of
input/output models, but the understanding of the constructions that student
have made from the information. Feedback is the information component whereas
reinforcement is the evaluative component relating to information and motivation.
For example:
• reinforcement
1.13
• corrective feedback .94
• remediation and feedback .65
• diagnosis feedback .52
• mastery learning (which is based on
feedback) .50
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 9Although not all
forms of feedback are as effective, for example:
• extrinsic rewards .37
• immediate vs. delayed .28
• punishment .20
There is a contrast here in the old fashioned notions of intrinsic and extrinsic rewards, although the better jargon now is task vs. ego involvement.
a. There are
many ways in which teachers can deliver feedback to students, and for students
to receive feedback from teachers, peers and other sources. The implication is
NOT that we should automatically use many tests and provide over-prescriptive
directions.
Rather, it means providing information how and why
the child understands and misunderstands, and what directions the student must
take to improve. Tests can perform this function but too often they are devoid
of much feedback to the students and thus can be an inefficient method of
providing feedback.
b. The incidence of feedback in the typical
classroom is very low, usually in seconds at best per day. Ask yourself how
much feedback there is in the typical lecture – yes, imparting information is
there, but how do you know it is appropriately challenging, that the student
shares or even understands the specific goals, and how do you provide
appropriate information about what directions the student must take to improve their
understanding. Perhaps it is no surprise to note that students who survive
university undergraduate degrees are those with impeccable surface and not deep
strategies, those who learn to be flexible to the instructors assessment
demands which so often value information and not understanding, and that the
students with the more deep, critical, and passionate learning strategies have
the highest probability of not completing our degrees.
c. It is likely that the information function of
feedback for an individual may be too diluted in a regular classroom to afford
much control over behaviour for special education students.
d. Reducing class size, prescribing more homework,
introducing more computers, etc. merely offers increased opportunities for more
feedback and appropriately challenging goals to occur - it does not guarantee
it occurs.
e. It is predicted that programs that do not
capitalize on effective classroom management practices to optimize feedback
will not be successful. One example is individualized instruction. Individualization
programs produce an average effect-size of .14, and programmed instruction
yields .18. Too often, individualization means placing the child alone to work
on a particular task, usually relating to his or her particular needs,
progress, pace, and behaviour. These attempts usually have little feedback,
little attention by a busy teacher catering to the other 30 or so students, and
the student typically has little knowledge of success or failure at the
specifics of the task. Teachers need excellent management systems with classes
greater than 20, and superb management systems with classes greater than 30. To
introduce individualization with 30 students means that, at best, a Inaugural
August 2, 1999 Page 10single student can only receive seconds of a teachers’
time per day, minutes of a teachers’ time in preparation, and merely passing glimpses
of time in feedback - no matter how noble the intentions of the teacher. It is
neither efficient nor effective to individualize instruction for 30 students,
expect high competence in the management of such programs, and provide feedback
to all students.
Individualization in regular classes must fail and
does fail. The negligible effects of individualization are particularly
important when it is recognised that students spend about 66 percent of their
time working alone (Rosenshine, 1979).
Feedback effects on performance are primarily via
cues that direct attention to the task-motivation processes, particularly when
coupled with information regarding erroneous hypothesis. The fewer the cognitive
resources needed for a task and the more the feedback is directed at the task
demands and not the person then the more powerful the feedback.
If we, as teachers, are to have an impact on
learning, then we must come to know what our students are thinking so that we
can provide more feedback, task information, encourage trial and error, and
develop deep understanding and transformations.
Third, achievement is enhanced to the degree that
students and teachers set challenging rather than "do your best"
goals relative to the students' present competencies. Goals need to be
specific, challenging, and it helps if the students are committed to these
goals -- although it is the challenge that is most critical (Locke &
Latham,1992).
Never allow a student to "do their best"
as this is the goal with the least challenge; everything the student does can
be claimed as the best! Appropriate, challenging, and specific goals inform
individuals "as to what type or level of performance is to be attained so
that they can direct and evaluate their actions and efforts accordingly.
Feedback allows them to set reasonable goals and to track their performance in
relation to their goals so that adjustments in effort, direction, and even
strategy can be made as needed" (Locke & Latham).
• They direct attention to relevant tasks or
outcomes
• They energise task performance
• They motivate individuals to persist in their
activities through time
• They convey normative information by suggesting
or specifying what level of performance the student could be expected to attain
• They have dramatic effects on the development of
self-efficacy, which in turn affects the choice of difficulty of goals
• Feedback without goal setting is less effective,
and goal setting without feedback is ineffective.
A combination of goal setting plus feedback is most
effective = goals and challenging goals are mutually supportive. The greater
the Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 11challenge the higher the probability of the
student seeking, receiving, and assimilating feedback information. The scenario
is that effective teachers set challenging goals and then structure situations
so that students can reach these goals. If teachers can encourage students to
share commitment to these challenging goals, and if they provide much feedback,
then goals are more likely to be attained.
A good example is the effectiveness of Outward
Bound programs to enhance self-esteem. Of the many self-esteem programs, one of
the most effective are the Outward Bound programs (Hattie, 1992). In these
programs, a common feature is the setting of seemingly very difficult goals
(e.g., abseiling), and then structuring the environment so that students can
attain these goals, while providing much informative feedback. Further, the
instructors remove many of the possibly irrelevant tasks so that students focus
on these challenging goals.
Fourth, it is teachers that make the difference.
Let me go back to the big picture, and re-order the effects into those that are
school policy and structural effects, social, home, what the student brings,
teacher methods, and learning process related. The bold are those effects
greater than the average. It is clear that structural and social influences are
minor, what the student brings in terms of achievement and disposition to learn
are powerful, teaching process are paramount, and the teacher methods are there
– this must lead to the conclusion that, yes teachers make the difference, but
only teachers who teach in certain ways.
Too many teachers compare what they are doing with
them not being there; they compare their methods with not using that method;
whereas teachers need to be more informed evaluators/consumers of teaching
methods. Like their students they must set challenging goals, seek feedback on
the effectiveness of their teaching on students, and constantly be attention to
improvement and innovating the methods which optimize feedback and meeting
challenging goals.
Fifth, the major argument is that most of the
structural innovations in schooling are probabilistic – their introduction
merely alter the probabilities of the core effects occurring: these core
effects include feedback, appropriate and challenging goals, and re-conceptualizations
as well as surface and deep knowledge.
Hence the five themes:
1. Innovation is the theme underlying most of these
effects;
2. The most powerful single moderator that enhances
achievement is feedback;
3. The setting of appropriate, specific and
challenging goals is critical;
4. It is what some teachers do that makes the
difference; and
5. The introduction of most teaching and school
influences merely impacts on the probability of the presence of feedback and
challenging goals.
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 13
Teachers must be
more informed evaluators as they need to ask how their methods increase the
probability of that which makes the difference – information, re-conceptualization,
feedback, and appropriate goals.
I must note that
• These findings along the continuum have
remarkable generality, across subjects, and ages. Generality is the norm.
• Aptitude treatment interactions are noted by
their absence. God must love main effects.
• Attempt to individualize instruction are not
noted by success.
Individualization programs produce an average
effect-size of .14, and programmed instruction yields .18. The negligible
effects of individualization are particularly important when it is recognized
that students spend about 2/3rds of their class time working alone.
• These studies have been undertaken primarily on
children in western countries. Academic achievement in low-income countries is
affected more by pupils' social status and less by school and teacher quality, whereas
the converse is the case in high-income countries like New Zealand.
• I have expanded this meta-analysis from
achievement to attitudes and a separate table for special education teachers –
with similar conclusions.
Thus, I hope I have
• Demonstrated the power of meta-analysis, and
research synthesis to address the more critical questions in education – I have
spent much research time synthesis these studies, finding major gaps and then filling
it with my own meta-analyses – for example on study skills, out- of-school
experiences, self-concept, and my current one is on the effects of ADHD/ADD
medication on learning
• Demonstrated the power of assessing magnitude
rather than statistical significance – which is one of my fascinations.
• And identified that which makes the difference on
student learning.
So far the prescriptions for influencing student
learning are clear: dollops of feedback, specific and challenging goals, and a
constant attention to asking, "How am I going".
There is at least one major complicating factor. The
inherent nature of learning is that there is a gap between the feedback and the
attainment of goals.
There are four possible ways for students to reduce
this gap:
• Students can increase their effort, although the
evidence we find of a "just-world" that more time in the class or
more time studying increases learning is not convincing.
• Students can abandon the standards, and they are
doing this in droves. We are successfully alienating so many students from our Inaugural
August 2, 1999 Page 14schools, particularly high schools as we promulgate the
merits of our subject cramming and chauvinism, while the students look to their
older peers who are successful in their eyes without that cramming.
We say students must have this knowledge about
English when most of the adult population does not need it; we say students
must have this advanced math when their fellow teachers do not have it and are successful.
Students become not engaged, and do not wish to have their reputations based on
attaining our social conformists goals of high school-based achievement;
• Students can change the standard by setting lower
goals in achievement at school, accepting far below their capabilities as satisfactory.
More often these drop out effects occur because students reject or re- interpret
the feedback information. It is this latter reaction that has so driven my
recent research program.
Students too often have conceptions of learning
inbred by years of shallow, fact-pushing, routine-ised teachers who pride
themselves on presenting the very best content, teaching from the front,
sitting tests, prodding students by external cues such as exams, and valuing
themselves in terms of successful imparting knowledge usually via teaching
models akin to drips into empty vessels.
Students, however, are not inert recipients and
build strategies to deal with this daily grind of the knowledge dump. Achievement
is enhanced to the degree that students develop self- strategies: to seek and
receive feedback to verify rather than enhance their sense of achievement
efficacy. Although I have spent many years exploring self-concept, this is NOT
what I am talking about. We have demonstrated there is a very low relationship between
self-concept and achievement (less then 4% of the variance), hence asking
causal questions is misplaced. We know much about the multidimensionality of
self-concept, and the age and gender effects etc. but this has rarely helped us
in our understanding of learning. More recently I have moved towards asking
about the relation between learning processes and self-strategies and
conceptions of learning. I do wish to emphasize that the self-strategies that
students have directly leads to enhanced or decreased performance AND their
conceptions of learning they have can lead them to (mis-) interpret the often
excellent teaching they receive – although I do not intend to purse the
conceptions of learning tonight.
There are two major self-strategies that students
used when learning:
Self status quo strategies
• the wish to be viewed as one believes one is;
• to do whatever it takes to preserve this concept
of self Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 15by adopting strategies that maximize
positive or minimize negative self- evaluations
Self testing strategies
• the seeking of confirmation and/or
disconfirmation about conceptions of self
• by adopting hypothesis-confirming strategies that
allow for the best opportunities for self-expression.
It is not so simple that we can match the use of
these strategies with high and low self-esteem, as all individuals prefer
self-verification but low self-esteem students have a greater tendency to use
self-enhancing strategies:
• When students are using self-verification
strategies, initial success signifies a talent or potential ability, whereas
for self-enhancement initial success confirms a deficiency needed to be remedied.
• Students using self-verification strategies cope
better with disconfirmation as they can relate to positive and negative
verifications of self. As a consequence of disconfirmation, those who use self-
verification make more optimistic predictions about their performance after
initial failure than after initial success, they seek specifically unfavorable
feedback so as to excel at the tasks.
• Students using self-enhancement show moderately
high persistence at the task after failure, consistent with the view that they
are interested in remedying their deficiencies in order to reach a passable
level of performance, which would afford them protection against humiliating failure.
Further, they tend to avoid tasks following initial success as such success signifies
that they have already reached an adequate level of performance, and further
tests merely run the risk of disconfirming the favorable outcome. This can lead
to rejection of success, not because they prefer failure but because success is
disconfirming, may require alterations to expectations, or maybe be threatening
because "they may lack confidence that they can repeat that success".
Yes, feedback is powerful, but the self-strategies
that students develop can alter the interpretation and consequences of this
feedback. Students self-enhance by biasing information and by selecting
information that provides affirmation of their prior beliefs. Providing
feedback to students is not enough
- as the ways and manner in which individuals
interpret your information is the key to developing positive and valuable
concepts of self. As has been
demonstrated by many researchers, including my own, this is where cultural
factors have a major mediating effect in the teaching-learning process. We have
undertaken these studies in Australia, Hong Kong, Japan, the USA, and with
tertiary, secondary, primary students, and more recently with groups of males
and groups of female juvenile delinquents and teenage prisoners. Understanding
their conceptions of learning and the self-strategies they have, and how they
use these to enhance their reputations is most powerful. I would conject that
there would be powerful self-strategies by many Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page
16Maori and Pacifica students that mediate the reception and assimilating of feedback
offered by Pakeha teachers.
We have been working with nine major strategies.
Nine major strategies
1. Self-handicapping
2. Discounting
3. Social comparison
4. Disconfirmation
5. Setting less challenging goals
6. Setting performance rather than task goals
7. Self-monitoring
8. Confirming negative cultural stereotypes
9. Seeking negative information
Self-handicapping providing a handicap that can be
used as an explanation for maintaining beliefs, and accounting for success or
failure that is inconsistent with prior beliefs. Example: a student could claim
he or she scored 100% on an examination because the items were too easy rather
than because of ability or effort in learning; or scored lower because of the
excellent television program they watched instead of studying.
Self-handicapping occurs when:
• students have high uncertainty about their
competencies
• there is high salience of an evaluative task
• in public rather than private performance
situations
• there is an abnormal investment in the question
of self-worth
• when the students believe that the handicap will
be viewed by others as a legitimate reason for potential failure It is used by
both high and low self-esteem individuals: high self-esteem people to enhance
success, low self-esteem people to protect themselves
against the threat of failure.
Discounting whereby feedback is
"dismissed" as being information that is not valuable, accurate, or
worthwhile for the individual. Example: when a teacher tells a student that he
or she is doing a great job, and the student’s reaction is to discount this by
claiming "she always says that," "she’s only trying to make me
feel good," or "it’s only because it is neat, not correct".
Social comparison whereby low self-esteem
individuals constantly monitor other peers’ behavior for cues and attributions
to explain/enhance their conceptions of self. They compare themselves with
others, and social comparison sets standards or frames of reference. Inaugural
August 2, 1999 Page 17Example: Students often compare themselves to those less
fortunate than themselves, and often attempt to present themselves as more
confident to impress others and maybe even themselves. Disconfirmation whereby
students ignore or reject information that disconfirms their self beliefs. Less
challenging goals whereby students set less challenging goals and thus ensure
more success and confirmation about their learning.
Setting performance rather than task goals.
Example, students who set performance goals are
more concerned with gaining favorable judgements of their competence, whereas
task goals are more concerned with increasing their competence on the task. Self-monitoring whereby students actively
plan, enact and guide their behavioral choices in social situations through the
process of self-monitoring. High self-monitors are more dictated to by the
external environment and by social comparison. Confirming negative cultural
stereotypes whereby students absorb negative societal stereotype about their
group’s intellectual ability and competence.
Steele (1992; Steele & Aronson, 1995), for
example, has argued that whenever African American students (particularly
males) perform an explicitly scholastic or intellectual task, they face the
threat of confirming or being judged by a negative societal stereotype about
their group’s intellectual ability and competence. Such a reputation influences
the academic functioning of these students, particularly during standardized
testing. He claimed that this reputation "may have the further effect of
pressuring these students to protectively disidentify with achievement in
school" (p. 797), such that school achievement is neither a basis of
self-evaluation nor a personal identity. There are various effects of this
cultural reputation (such as spending more time answering fewer test items)
that can undermine motivation, effort, and self-efficacy.
Seeking on negative information.
Although all
students prefer favorable feedback some have adverse stress reactions to
negative feedback – hence try to avoid seeking negative information altogether
and seek only positive information.
The effects of the strategies
These self learning strategies explain how
individuals can bias, select and retain information that affect their
self-concepts and they have major influences on how students learn, how they
set challenging goals, how they accept feedback, and their subsequent learning
outcomes. The point is that a teacher may be providing a remarkable amount of feedback
but this does not mean that this student is receiving this feedback. Inaugural
August 2, 1999 Page 18Excellent teaching requires teachers setting challenging
goals and providing feedback, and students setting challenging goals, and
seeking, biasing and interpreting information and accepting feedback. Oh, not
so easy, eh! If only it was one-way.
Teacher Education
The implications of the above have much to say to
teacher education. Teachers need to be well prepared in their content to know
WHAT information is to be taught, and WHAT information can be used as feedback–clearly if they have deeper understandings of
their content they increase the probability they can set challenging goals and
provide feedback. More important teachers need to understand that the
variations in individual students manner of receiving, transforming and being
disposed to this information is considered in the act of teaching. This, I
believe, requires that teachers care about their students, know where they are
coming from, and overly attend to the issue of whether their students are
learning.
For example, we have been researching the Paideia
teaching method, which was forcibly introduced into every class in 91 schools.
Yes, it was innovation, yes it was a teacher method and not a structural
change, and yes, it involved setting challenging goals and dramatic amounts of
feedback – all the hallmarks for success; but forcibly introduced.
The Paideia program promotes three modes of
teaching:
a didactic mode, a seminar component, and a coaching aspect. The
seminar component is the most distinctive and involves much training in active
listening by the teacher, constructing peer group tutoring for the whole class
that uses rich and challenging texts (across all the curricula), and structures
ways for the students to question the text and each others understandings. I
used Paideia in my classes in measurement and statistics and it opened up to
me, as teacher, the ways in which the students were constructing knowledge, and
the ways they had already constructed it – very sobering – despite my having taught
the material using the drip filter model perfectly.
In the forced implementation there was remarkable
success in the standardised test scores across all ages, subjects, and most
critically in this particular county for all racial backgrounds, primarily by
reducing the negative effects of social comparison that African American
students had been using. There was also powerful evidence that Paideia
influenced students’ perceptions of the quality of schooling. The students claimed
that teachers who implemented Paideia were better at explaining information,
more able in ensuring that students had a good understanding, put more effort
into teaching, taught in interesting ways, and showed by example that learning
is fun. There was less friction in classes, less fooling around, students were considered
more calm and not mean, and they felt safe. Students see more flexibility in
the classroom when Paideia is implemented. Teacher Education programs must
produce teachers with a conception of teaching based on a high and rigorous set
of standards of what beginning Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 19teachers know
and are able to do. I have worked with NCATE developing a new model for teacher
accreditation based on this simple notion. Instead of asking institutions, like
ours here at UoA, what courses do your students experience, and are the staff
qualified.
We oversaw:
• a major consultative process to determine the
high and rigorous competencies of graduating teachers in terms of what they
know, and are able to do
• asked the Institutions to provide evidence that
their graduating teachers know and able to do the competencies to the
appropriate standards of excellence, and
• ask the professionals to adjudge this evidence.
We are in trials in many Institutions in the US at
the moment and this is beginning to have a profound and positive effect on what
is undertaken in teacher education institutions. No longer is exposure to
wisdom sufficient, no longer are scores on our student sufficient, no longer
are our posturing that we are quality providers convincing. Instead, the course
is evaluated by the quality of the graduates in terms of knowing, performing
and their dispositions in terms of their impact on their students’ learning
outcomes– a hard but correct ask.
We spent much time in teacher education programs
imparting the necessary knowledge so that students are sufficiently learn to
teach, we spent much time on teaching methods, and we need to spend much time
understanding how students learn, their conceptions of learning, and how the
feedback cycle works. Too often we compared experienced with novice when the
better comparison is experienced and expert. This is my mission.
Identifying Accomplished Teachers
We have a system that spends inordinate energies
worrying about initial teacher training when more attention needs to be placed
at the highest ends of excellence. We have a model of teaching which assumes
that experience is sufficient, we pride well run quiet classrooms, we pride
mimicking, listening, and regurgitating information. I have argued extensively
elsewhere that New Zealand needs a National Board for Professional Teaching
Standards.
I propose the development of an Australasian Board
for Professional Education Standards (ABPES) that parallels the USA National
Board (NBPTS) to advance the excellence of the principal and teaching
profession.
The mission would be to:
• Establish high and rigorous standards for what
accomplished teachers and principals should know and be able to do,
• Operate a national voluntary system to assess and
certify teachers and principals who meet these standards, and
• To advance related education reforms for the
purpose of improving student learning in Australasian schools.
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 20This has been done
in the USA and I am still involved in projects relating to the National Board.
Currently, I have a $.75m project on how NBCT and non- NBCT teachers manifest
their expertise in the classroom, and not surprisingly given tonight, are finding
differences relating to the amount of feedback, the manner the teacher
understands the learner and how he or she is learning, deals with the
connections of learning and strategies of learning, and cares and respects what
the child brings to their classroom. These facets are often missing in the
too-typical over-routines classroom where teaching is undertaken as knowledge
forced into the recalcitrant brains, assembled in straight rows, silent,
listening and waiting to be tested. A major imperative why we must identify,
reward, and promote these excellent teachers in a fair, credible and dependable
manner is that they are
most likely to provide the voting public the
confidence in a national system of public schooling. National testing, braying
by parents and politicians about low quality and more accountability, and the
highlighting of the minimally competent reinforces those 80% of New Zealanders
who typically argue that the quality of public schooling in New Zealand is
awful; whereas the identification of excellence reinforces those 80% of New
Zealanders who believe that the quality of their child’s teacher is great. We
must promote excellence.
Research Program
To conclude I wish to outline a series of research
study that I and my colleagues (and I hope colleagues here in Auckland) have
underway:
What influences student learning
• A model of teaching and learning based on a
synthesis of meta- analyses
From the student perspective
• Defending the power of feedback, and goals (in
classrooms, out-of- classrooms such as Outward Bound, Adventure Sailing ships,
and with at-risk students)
• Developing models of conceptions of learning of
teachers and students and how these affect how teachers teach and students
learn
• Discovering self strategies for learning (particularly
cross-culturally)
• Building models of conceptions of self
particularly based on reputation enhancement
• Ascertaining the study skills of students that
assist in the learning process
From the teacher perspective
• Determining the differences between experienced
and expert teachers
• Assessing the teaching methods that increase the
probability of impact on student learning
• Devising measurement procedures to address the
question as to how
expert and experienced teachers differ in classroom
practice and how to identify highly accomplished teachers
• Developing accreditation processes for teacher
education programs
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 21• The development
of a New Zealand National Board that sets high and
rigorous standards and has excellent performance
measures to identify accomplished teachers
Addressing the desirable outcomes
• Tests and measures of models of achievement,
particularly surface and deep outcomes based on the SOLO model
• Tests and measures of learning self-strategies
• Training teachers in setting standards (e.g.,
using exemplars, scoring rubrics)
• Developing models of wellness to add to the
achievement outcomes Using the best statistical modeling
• Conducting meta-analyses to achieve integration,
perspective, and magnitude of effects
• Incorporating measures of magnitude with
statistical significance in research designs
• Building structural modelling to assess
explanations and prediction
• Using item response models to assess
dimensionality and create better measurement, particularly in the affective
domain
• Developing and conducting evaluation models of
successful teaching and school innovations I now consider myself inaugurated.
Selected Bibliography
Carroll, A., Durkin, K., Hattie, J., &
Houghton, S. (1997). Goal setting among adolescents: A comparison of
delinquent, at-risk, and not at-risk youth.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 441-450. Carroll,
A., Durkin, K., Houghton, S., & Hattie, J.A. (1996). An adaptation of Mak’s
self-reported delinquency scale for Western Australian adolescents. Australian
Journal of Psychology, 48, 1-7.
Carroll, A., Durkin, K., Houghton, S., &
Hattie, J.A. (1996). An adaptation of Mak’s self-reported delinquency scale for
Western Australian adolescents. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 1-7.
Carroll, A., Hattie, J.A., Douglas, G. (in press).
Adolescent reputation enhancement: Differentiating delinquent and nondelinquent
youths. European Journal of Social Psychology. Carroll, A.M., Hattie, J.A.,
Houghton, S., & Durkin, K. (in press). Reputation Enhancing Goals:
Integrating reputation enhancement and goal setting theory as an explanation of
delinquent involvement.
Cheung, D., Hattie, J.A., Bucat, R., & Douglas,
G. (1996). Students’ perceptions of curriculum implementation. Curriculum and
Teaching, 11, 49-60.
Cheung, D., Hattie, J.A., Bucat, R., & Douglas,
G. (1996). Measuring the degree of implementation of school-based assessment
schemes for practical science. Research in Science Education, 26, 375-389.
Crane, D., Hattie, J.A., & Houghton, S. (1997).
Goal setting and the Adventure Experience. Australian Journal of Psychology,
49, 6-13.
Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 22Dimmock, C. &
Hattie, J. (1992). Principals’ and teachers’ reactions to school restructuring:
The case of Western Australia. Australian Journal of Education, 38, 38-57.
Dimmock, C. & Hattie, J. (1994). Principals’
and teachers’ reactions to school restructuring. Australian Journal of
Education, 38, 36-55.
Dimmock, C., & Hattie, J. (1996). School
principals’ self-efficacy and its measurement in a context of restructuring.
School Effectiveness and School Improvement: An International Journal of
Research, Policy, and Practice, 7, 62-75.
Elliott, E., Hattie, J.A., et al. (1998). Program
Standards for Elementary teacher preparation. National Council for
Accreditation of Teacher Education, Washington, DC. Forlin, C., Douglas, G.,
Hattie, J. (1996). Inclusive practices: Is it stressful for teachers?
International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 21, 199-217.
Fraser, B., Walberg, H., Welch, W., & Hattie,
J.A. (1987). Syntheses of educational productivity research. Monograph in
International Journal of Educational Research Series. Oxford: Pergamon. pp.
252.
Hattie, J. (1992). The levels of school
accountability. Education Research and Perspectives, 19, 78-80.
Hattie, J. (1995). Rural schools and student
outcomes. In Schooling in Rural WA: Report of the Ministerial Review of
Schooling in Rural Western Australia. WA Government.
Hattie, J.A. & Jaeger, R.M. (1998). Assessment
and learning. Assessment in Higher Education, 5, 111-122.
Hattie, J.A. & Marsh, H.W. (1995). Future
research in self-concept. In B. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook on Self-concept (pp.
421-463). Erlbaum, NJ.Hattie, J.A. & Purdie, N. (1998). The SOLO method and
item construction. In G. Boulton-Lewis & B. Dart (Eds.), Learning in Higher
Education.
Hawthorn, Australia: ACER. Hattie, J.A. (1992).
Self-concept. Erlbaum, USA, pp. 304.
Hattie, J.A. (1992). Towards a model of schooling:
A synthesis of meta- analyses. Australian Journal of Education, 36, 5-13.
Hattie, J.A. (1993). Measuring the effects of
schooling. SET, 2, 1-4.
Hattie, J.A. (1997). Setting standards for
beginning teachers: A discussion paper. National Council for Accreditation of
Teaching Standards, Washington DC.
Hattie, J.A. Clinton, J.C., Nagle, B., Kelkor, V.,
Reid, W., Spence, K., Baker, W., & Jaeger, R. (1998). The first year
evaluation of Paideia. Bryan Foundation and Guilford County Schools.
Hattie, J.A., & Clinton, J.C. (in press).
Identifying Accomplished Teachers: A Validation Study. In L. Invargasson,
(Ed.), Assessing teachers for professional Certification: The first ten years
of the National Board for Professional Teaching Standards. JAI Press.
Hattie, J.A., & Haines, L. (1998). An Evaluation
of the Reading Together, USA program. Greensboro, NC. Hattie, J.A., &
Jaeger, R.M. (1998).The construct validity of NBPTS assessments and
classifications of candidate teachers. NPEAT, Washington, DC.
Hattie, J.A., & Purdie, N.M. (in press).
Individual differences. International Encyclopedia of Educational Psychology. Inaugural
August 2, 1999 Page 23Hattie, J.A., Biggs, J., & Purdie, N. (1996). Effects
of learning skills intervention on student learning: A meta-analysis. Review of
Research in Education, 66, 99-136.
Hattie, J.A., Fletcher, R., Karkee, T. (1996). An
analysis of school suspensions in Guilford County Schools. Confidential report
presented to the School Board, Guilford County, NC. Hattie, J.A., Jaeger, R.M.,
& Bond, L. (in press). Pervasive problems in educational measurement.
Review of Research in Education.
Hattie, J.A., Kefford, R., & Porter, P. (1988).
Skills, Technology, and Management in Education: The Dynamics of Learning.
(Eds.) Australian College of Education, Melbourne, 1988, Deakin, ACT. pp. 141.
Hattie, J.A., Krakowski, K., Rogers, H.J., &
Swaminathan, H. (1996). An assessment of Stout’s index of essential
unidimensionality. Applied Psychological Measurement, 20, 1-14.
Hattie, J.A., Marsh, H.W., Neill, J.T., &
Richards, G.E. (1997). Adventure Education and outward Bound: Out-of-class
experiences that make a lasting difference. Review of Research in Education,
66, 43-87.
Hattie. J.A. (in press). Validating the
specification of a complex content domain: Applications to the National Board
for Professional teaching Standards’ Assessments. In L. Invargasson, (Ed.),
Assessing teachers for professional Certification: The first ten years of the
National Board for Professional Teaching Standards. JAI Press. Hoffman, R.M.,
Border, L.D., & Hattie, J.A. (in press). Gender self-confidence: An
alternative approach to the measurement of masculinity and femininity. Sex
Roles.
Jaeger, R. M. & Hattie, J. A. (1996). Artifact
and artifice in education policy analysis: It’s not all in the data. The School
Administrator, 53, 24-29.
Jaeger, R.M., & Hattie, J.A. (1995, November).
Detracking America’s Schools: Should we really care? Phi Delta Kappan, 77(3),
218-219.
Law, H.G., Snyder, C.W., Hattie, J.A. &
McDonald, R.P. (1984). Multi-mode models for data analysis. Praeger, USA. pp.
687.
Maguire, T.O.M. & Hattie, J.A., & Haig, B.
(1994) Construct validity and achievement assessment. Alberta Journal of
Educational Research, 40, 109-126.
Marsh, H.W., & Hattie, J.A. (1995). Theoretical
models in self-concept. In B. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook on Self-concept. (pp.
38-92). Erlbaum, NJ. Moriarty, B., Punch, K., & Hattie, J. (1995). The
importance of self-efficacy as a mediating variable between learning
environments and achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65,
73-84.
Purdie, N., & Hattie, J. (1995). The effect of
motivation training on approaches to learning and self-concept. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 65, 73-81.
Purdie, N., & Hattie, J.A. (1996). Cultural
differences in the use of strategies for self-regulated learning. American
Educational Research Journal, 33, 845-871.
Purdie, N., Hattie. J.A., & Douglas, G. (1996).
Student conceptions of learning and their use of self-regulated learning
strategies: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88,
87-100.
Watkins, D., Hattie, J.A., & Astilla, E.
(1990). Affective and cognitive learning outcomes in privileged and depressed
areas of the Philippines. In A.H. Otheman & W.R.A. Rahman (Eds.),
Psychology and socioeconomic Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 24development (pp.
203-217). Penerb and Universiti Kebangsaan, Malaysia, Bangi.
Watkins, D., Hattie, J.A., & Regmi, M. (1994).
The structure of self-esteem in Nepalese children. Psychological Reports, 74,
832-839. Inaugural August 2, 1999 Page 25
0 comments:
Posting Komentar